Excel At Life--Dedicated to the Pursuit of Excellence in Life, Relationships, Sports and Career
Excel At Life logo
×

Excel At Life
Contents

Home

Apps

Cognitive Diary Examples

Passive-Aggressive Q&A

PsychNotes

Topics

Anxiety

CBT

Depression

Conflict

Goal Setting

Happiness

Jealousy

Motivation

Relationships

Self-esteem

SportPsych

Wellness

CBT Jealousy Depression Relationships Conflict Self-efficacy Happiness Goal-setting Motivation Wellness Sport Psych

Popular Articles

Crazy-Makers: Dealing with Passive-Aggressive People

Why Are People Mean? Don't Take It Personally!

When You Have Been Betrayed

Struggling to Forgive: An Inability to Grieve

Happy Habits: 50 Suggestions

The Secret of Happiness: Let It Find You (But Make the Effort)

Excellence vs. Perfection

Depression is Not Sadness

20 Steps to Better Self-Esteem

7 Rules and 8 Methods for Responding to Passive-aggressive People

What to Do When Your Jealousy Threatens to Destroy Your Marriage

Happiness is An Attitude

Guide to How to Set Achieveable Goals

Catastrophe? Or Inconvenience?

Popular Audios

Panic Assistance

Motivational Audios

Mindfulness Training

Rational Thinking

Relaxation for Children

Loving Kindness Meditation

Self-Esteem Exercise

Lies You Were Told

Choosing Happiness

Audio Version of Article: Crazy-Makers: Passive-Aggressive People

Audio Version of Article: Why Are People Mean? Don't Take It Personally!

Audio Version of Article: Happiness Is An Attitude

All Audio Articles

PsychNotes Index

More PsychNotes: Personality

When Do Good People Cheat? Or, Developing Compassion by Recognizing Your Flaws
by Monica A. Frank, PhD

Not surprisingly, a state of physical deprivation can lead to violating personal moral beliefs if it reduces the physical distress. Research showed that when people had an opportunity to cheat without getting caught they were more likely to do so if they were hungry or thirsty and the reward was food or water. On the other hand, if the reward was not something that would reduce their physical discomfort, they were not more likely to cheat (Williams et al., 2016).

These researchers conducted their studies with the average consumer in a grocery store (hunger group) or with someone who had just worked out in a gym (thirsty group). The participants were not chronically deprived but were in a temporary state of hunger or thirst. If asked, most people say they would not violate their moral beliefs for small pay-offs. Yet, this research showed that minor physical need can cause a person to cheat to reduce their immediate discomfort.

Several conclusions can be drawn from this research:

1) Good intentions can be overridden by discomfort. For example, people who are on a diet are often told not to go to the grocery store when they are hungry because it is more difficult to resist bad choices. The violation of a moral principle of honesty may be justified by some people when it promises an immediate improvement in comfort level. “What does it matter if I cheat? It's not hurting anybody.”

2) Physical discomfort can be difficult to resist relieving immediately. This research was done with people who were in states of minor discomfort. Imagine how difficult it must be for people struggling with ongoing deprivation. If many of us have trouble resisting minor discomfort to the extent we are willing to cheat, what must it be like for those who are truly deprived?

3) Morality varies depending upon perspective. People often look down on others who don't behave according to society's standards and yet justify similar behavior of their own. However, compassion for the moral flaws of others comes from recognizing that most of us struggle with morality. We may strive to be moral but when simple things such as temporary hunger or thirst can affect our moral behavior, we need to recognize the difficulty of maintaining moral beliefs. When we can see our own flaws we may be less likely to hold others in contempt because they don't meet our standards. By doing so we can have greater compassion for those in chronic need.

Williams, E. F., Pizarro, D., Ariely, D., & Weinberg, J. D. (2016, May 5). The Valjean Effect: Visceral States and Cheating. Emotion. Advance online publication. DOI: 10.1037/emo0000158


curved line